Home | About IPJ | Editorial board | Ahead of print | Current Issue | Archives | Instructions | Contact us |   Login 
Industrial Psychiatry Journal
Search Articles   
    
Advanced search   
 


 
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year : 2017  |  Volume : 26  |  Issue : 2  |  Page : 178-182  Table of Contents     

Neuropsychological deficits in elderly with depression


1 Department of Psychiatry, BMHRC, Bhopal, Madhya Pradesh, India
2 Department of Psychology, Patna University, Patna, India
3 Department of Psychiatry, People's Medical College, Bhanpur, Bhopal, Madhya Pradesh, India
4 Department of Psychiatry, Dr D Y Patil Medical College, Pimpri, Pune, Maharashtra, India

Date of Web Publication10-Jul-2018

Correspondence Address:
Dr. Rupesh Ranjan
Department of Psychiatry, Bhopal Memorial Hospital and Research Center, Bhopal, Madhya Pradesh
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/ipj.ipj_86_14

Rights and Permissions
   Abstract 


Background: Depressed patients are preoccupied with unhappy thoughts which reduce their capacity to focus on attention, memory, and other cognitive performance. Aim: The aim of this study is to assess neuropsychological deficits in elderly depressive and compare it with matched normal controls. Methods: After consideration of inclusion and exclusion criteria, the sample of 30 elderly depressive patients diagnosed on the basis of International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Edition criteria and 30 normal controls were selected. The selection of sample was by purposive sampling from private psychiatric clinic of Bhopal. The age range of sample was 60 years and above. All participants were administered the Geriatric Depression Scale, and the Luria–Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery-1 (LNNB form-1). Results: On the Geriatric Depression Scale, 21 patients were at mild level and nine patients were at severe level of depression. None of the normal controls were depressed. On LNNB form-1, depressive patients showed significant elevation on receptive speech, arithmetic, memory, reading, writing, and expressive speech as compared to normal controls. Conclusion: Older depressive patients showed significantly more neurocognitive deficits as compared to normal controls. It is important that these deficits are identified and addressed for the holistic treatment of late-onset depression.

Keywords: Geriatric depression, Luria Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery form-1, neuropsychological deficits


How to cite this article:
Ranjan R, Priyamvada R, Jha GK, Chaudhury S. Neuropsychological deficits in elderly with depression. Ind Psychiatry J 2017;26:178-82

How to cite this URL:
Ranjan R, Priyamvada R, Jha GK, Chaudhury S. Neuropsychological deficits in elderly with depression. Ind Psychiatry J [serial online] 2017 [cited 2019 Sep 19];26:178-82. Available from: http://www.industrialpsychiatry.org/text.asp?2017/26/2/178/236199



The ageing process is a biological reality which has its own dynamic, largely beyond human control. Old age is a phase of the life cycle characterized by its own developmental issues, many of which are concerned with loss of physical agility, and mental acuity, friends and loved ones, and status and power. Internationally, the prevalence of depression and cognitive impairment in geriatric participants is estimated to be 11%–30%[1],[2] and 17%–36%,[2],[3],[4] respectively. Therefore, though depression is primarily a mood disorder, for many older adults, it is also a cognitive disorder. Elderly depressives with comorbid cognitive impairment are a particular clinical concern because it increases the rate of adverse outcomes for physical health, functional status, and mortality.[5]

There has been a renewal of interest in testing patients' with depression on a broad range of neuropsychological tasks in the last decade. This has promoted a growing awareness that, like schizophrenia and other neurological disorders, mood disorders may be associated with a distinct pattern of cognitive impairment. Such impairments of cognitive function are seldom measured. This is surprising because it is easier objectively to measure memory impairment, for example, than it is to measure other core feature of depression in old age such as the severity of depressed mood or sleep disturbance. However, also central to current interest is the effort to link theories of cognitive neuropsychology to the anatomy and physiology of brain function. If depression is indeed a brain disease, then neuropsychological impairments may lead us to the relevant neural substrates. It is now commonly accepted that depression with age is associated with a number of deficits in episodic memory and learning.[6] This finding is consistent across most studies and appears to involve both explicit verbal and visual memory in patients with both melancholic endogenous and nonmelancholic nonendogenous depression.[7] Implicit memory tasks, on the other hand, appear to be spared.[8],[9],[10],[11] Temporal lobe lesions typically disrupt episodic memory; given that reductions in hippocampal volume are demonstrated in patients with major depression, it may be that impaired mnemonic function is associated with dysfunction of the hippocampus in depression.[12] Depression with cognitive impairment is among the most important mental health problem which occurs in elderly people. In both conditions, the patients have severe consequences, including diminished quality of life, functional decline, increased use of services, and high mortality.

Late-onset depression and cognitive impairment often occur together, suggesting a close association between them. It is not known, however, whether depression leads to cognitive decline or vice versa. Clinical practice and research evidence suggest that depression precedes cognitive decline in old age. However, inferring a relationship is hampered because most studies on this topic examined only the association between depression and the subsequent development of cognitive impairment. Thus, the present study was undertaken to assess the neuropsychological deficits in elderly depressive and compare it with matched normal controls.


   Materials and Methods Top


Sample

The sample consisted of thirty elderly depressive patients diagnosed on the basis of International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Edition-Diagnostic Criteria for Research [13] criteria and age range of 60 and above with minimum of 2 years of illness and free from comorbid psychiatric or major medical illnesses. Equal number of age- and sex-matched control participants without any psychiatric or physical illnesses formed the control group. All participants were explained the aim of the study and gave informed consent.

Sampling technique

Sampling technique was purposive sampling. Patients were selected from private clinics of Bhopal.

Tools used for assessment

  • Sociodemographic and clinical datasheet
  • Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS)[14]
  • The Luria–Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery (LNNB, form-1).[15]


Procedure

After screening according to inclusion and exclusion criteria, patients were selected for study. Clinical interview and required history were taken and sociodemographic sheet was filled. GDS was administered to assess the depression in elderly. LNNB-I was administered in 2–3 sessions over a period of 1 day of 2 consecutive days on both depressive and normal controls.

The tests were scored as per the test manuals. Statistical analyses were carried out using Chi-square test and Student's t-test as appropriate.


   Results Top


The mean and standard deviation (SD) of age of normal and depression patients was 62.66 (±2.08) years and 63.46 (±2.40) years, respectively. The mean and SD of education level of normal and depressive patients was 13.2 ± 2.97 years and 11.8 ± 3.42 years, respectively. All the participants were male, married, from urban area, and right handed. There were no significant differences between the depressive patients and normal controls on the sociodemographic characteristics.

On the GDS, 21 patients were at mild level and nine patients were at severe level of depression. None of the normal controls were depressed. The mean critical level of LNNB-I profile of normal and depressive was 61.9 and 64.23 levels, respectively. On LNNB-I depressed elderly participants showed significantly poorer performance than normal on receptive speech, writing, reading, arithmetic, and memory [Table 1].
Table 1: Comparison between elderly depression and matched control subjects on Luria–Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery-I

Click here to view



   Discussion Top


The study reveals that depressive patients underwent a general cognitive decline which is evidenced by differences in neuropsychological test performance relative to normal controls on LNNB-I; performance differences were especially in domain of receptive speech, reading, writing, and memory. The poor performance of depressive patients compared to the normal controls in the present study indicates that probably depressive symptoms are associated with cognitive decline which is supported by the findings of earlier studies.[16],[17] A meta-analytic study also concluded that a motivation deficit has the potential to impair the performance of all neuropsychological tasks in depression.[18]

Cognitive functions are associated with symptoms of depression. Functional imaging and brain stimulation studies have convincingly proved that dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VmPFC) play significant roles in the pathophysiology of depression. Apart from playing an important role in the generation of negative emotion, through its projections to amygdala and hypothalamus, VmPFC also coordinates the negative emotions.[19] Studies suggest that DLPFC-mediated cognitive control functions, memory, learning, attention executive functions, also pertain to emotion. Specifically, functional imaging studies demonstrate the recruitment of DLPFC during the regulation of negative emotion through reappraisal/suppression strategies.[20] Studies associate depression with abnormally high levels of VmPFC activity [21],[22] but abnormally low levels of DLPFC activity,[23],[24],[25] thus affecting cognitive functions. This could explain the findings of the present study where patients with depression showed poor performance on LNNB-I scale.

The mean critical level of LNNB-I profile of depressive and normal were 64.2 and 61.9 levels, respectively. On LNNB-I, depressive patients show significantly poorer performance than normal controls which is clear from [Table 1] which indicates that LNNB-I profile of depressive shows elevation on scales above the critical level, namely, receptive speech, writing, reading, arithmetic, and memory. However, LNNB-I profile of normal participants shows scale above the critical levels, namely, arithmetic, memory, and intellectual scales. Thus, it was found from the present study that although there was elevation on mean profile of both the groups, a significant difference was obtained between both the groups only on receptive speech, writing, reading, and memory scales. The above findings are consistent with the findings of an earlier study which found that in comparison to controls, depression is associated with a number of deficits in episodic memory and learning.[26] Another recent study also noted deficit in immediate verbal memory, short-term memory, and working memory of major depressive patients with increasing age.[27] Earlier studies have also reported that depressives showed impairments in complex integration for concept formation, spontaneous cognitive flexibility, initiation ability,[28] and have poor performance strategies in comparison with controls.[29]

In the present study, depressive had poor performance than normal control on memory functions and also had problem in new learning and recall interference which indicates that during depressive stage, patients are unable to carry out cognitive functions properly. This is consistent with findings of a recent study which found a significant variation in performance between depressive patients and normal controls in most cognitive functions, especially memory (P < 0.0001), semantic fluency (P < 0.0001), verbal fluency, and digit symbol (P < 0.0001). Late-onset depression patients scored lower and exhibited more severe impairment in memory domains than early-onset depression patients (P < 0.05). Both depressed groups, early and late onset, were more inactive than controls (P < 0.05; odds ratio: 6.02).[30] Depressive symptoms may be a prodrome of cognitive decline, the early manifestation of a neurodegenerative process, causing depression, and dementia. Depression with onset after 60 years of age is associated with pathology of subcortical and deep white matter structures [31] and is more frequently associated with cognitive impairment, typically with prominent abnormalities in executive function that reflect underlying disruption of frontostriatal circuits.[32],[33],[34],[35]


   Conclusion Top


Elderly patients with depression have significantly more neurocognitive deficits as compared to age-matched nondepressed controls. It is important that these deficits are identified and addressed for the holistic treatment of late-onset depression.

Limitations

The study had various limitations. The sample size was small. Females could not be included in the study which is a primary limitation in the applicability of the present findings to female depressive patients. A structured clinical interview was not conducted for making psychiatric diagnosis. Being a cross-sectional study, we could not conclude whether depression leads to cognitive dysfunction or vice versa. Neuroradiological investigations would have added to better inference.

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
   References Top

1.
Copeland JR, Beekman AT, Braam AW, Dewey ME, Delespaul P, Fuhrer R, et al. Depression among older people in Europe: The EURODEP studies. World Psychiatry 2004;3:45-9.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Lee Y, Shinkai S. Correlates of cognitive impairment and depressive symptoms among older adults in Korea and Japan. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005;20:576-86.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Rait G, Fletcher A, Smeeth L, Brayne C, Stirling S, Nunes M, et al. Prevalence of cognitive impairment: Results from the MRC trial of assessment and management of older people in the community. Age Ageing 2005;34:242-8.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Graciani A, Banegas JR, Guallar-Castillón P, Domínguez-Rojas V, Rodríguez-Artalejo F. Cognitive assessment of the non-demented elderly community dwellers in Spain. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:104-12.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Mehta KM, Yaffe K, Langa KM, Sands L, Whooley MA, Covinsky KE, et al. Additive effects of cognitive function and depressive symptoms on mortality in elderly community-living adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2003;58:M461-7.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Goodwin GM, Austin MP, Dougall N, Ross M, Murray C, O'Carroll RE, et al. State changes in brain activity shown by the uptake of 99mTc-exametazime with single photon emission tomography in major depression before and after treatment. J Affect Disord 1993;29:243-53.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Austin MP, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K, Parker G, Hickie I, Brodaty H, et al. Cognitive function in depression: A distinct pattern of frontal impairment in melancholia? Psychol Med 1999;29:73-85.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Hertel PT, Hardin TS. Remembering with and without awareness in a depressed mood: Evidence of deficits in initiative. J Exp Psychol Gen 1990;119:45-59.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Denny EB, Hunt RR. Affective valence and memory in depression: Dissociation of recall and fragment completion. J Abnorm Psychol 1992;101:575-80.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Danion JM, Kauffmann-Muller F, Grangé D, Zimmermann MA, Greth P. Affective valence of words, explicit and implicit memory in clinical depression. J Affect Disord 1995;34:227-34.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Ilsley JE, Moffoot AP, O'Carroll RE. An analysis of memory dysfunction in major depression. J Affect Disord 1995;35:1-9.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Sheline YI, Wang PW, Gado MH, Csernansky JG, Vannier MW. Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1996;93:3908-13.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders, Diagnostic criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO; 1993.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 1982;17:37-49.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Golden CJ, Purish AD, Hammeke TA. Luria –Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery: Forms I & II (Manual). Los Angeles Western Psychological Services; 1985.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Köhler S, Thomas AJ, Barnett NA, O'Brien JT. The pattern and course of cognitive impairment in late-life depression. Psychol Med 2010;40:591-602.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Priyamvada R, Jahan M. Cross validity of 22-items and 15-items screening tests from LNNB-1on depressive patients. Indian J Clin Psychol 2009;36:84-7.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Austin MP, Mitchell P, Goodwin GM. Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible implications for functional neuropathology. Br J Psychiatry 2001;178:200-6.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Ongür D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex 2000;10:206-19.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, Gabrieli JD. Rethinking feelings: An FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14:1215-29.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Drevets WC, Videen TO, Price JL, Preskorn SH, Carmichael ST, Raichle ME, et al. A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression. J Neurosci 1992;12:3628-41.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Mayberg HS, Lozano AM, Voon V, McNeely HE, Seminowicz D, Hamani C, et al. Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Neuron 2005;45:651-60.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Biver F, Goldman S, Delvenne V, Luxen A, De Maertelaer V, Hubain P, et al. Frontal and parietal metabolic disturbances in unipolar depression. Biol Psychiatry 1994;36:381-8.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Baxter LR Jr., Schwartz JM, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC, Guze BH, Selin CE, et al. Reduction of prefrontal cortex glucose metabolism common to three types of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46:243-50.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.
Galynker II, Cai J, Ongseng F, Finestone H, Dutta E, Serseni D, et al. Hypofrontality and negative symptoms in major depressive disorder. J Nucl Med 1998;39:608-12.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.
Goodwin GM. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal lobes in depression. J Psychopharmacol 1997;11:115-22.  Back to cited text no. 26
    
27.
Mondal S, Sharma VK, Das S, Goswami U, Gandhi A. Neurocognitive function in patients of major depression. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2007;51:69-75.  Back to cited text no. 27
    
28.
Fossati P, Amar G, Raoux N, Ergis AM, Allilaire JF. Executive functioning and verbal memory in young patients with unipolar depression and schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1999;89:171-87.  Back to cited text no. 28
    
29.
Channon S, Green PS. Executive function in depression: The role of performance strategies in aiding depressed and non-depressed participants. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;66:162-71.  Back to cited text no. 29
    
30.
Dillon C, Allegri RF, Serrano CM, Iturry M, Salgado P, Glaser FB, et al. Late- versus early-onset geriatric depression in a memory research center. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2009;5:517-26.  Back to cited text no. 30
    
31.
Krishnan KR, Hays JC, Blazer DG. MRI-defined vascular depression. Am J Psychiatry 1997;154:497-501.  Back to cited text no. 31
    
32.
Alexopoulos GS, Kiosses DN, Klimstra S, Kalayam B, Bruce ML. Clinical presentation of the “depression-executive dysfunction syndrome” of late life. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002;10:98-106.  Back to cited text no. 32
    
33.
Butters MA, Whyte EM, Nebes RD, Begley AE, Dew MA, Mulsant BH, et al. The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61:587-95.  Back to cited text no. 33
    
34.
Potter GG, Steffens DC. Contribution of depression to cognitive impairment and dementia in older adults. Neurologist 2007;13:105-17.  Back to cited text no. 34
    
35.
Thomas P, Hazif Thomas C, Billon R, Peix R, Faugeron P, Clément JP, et al. Depression and frontal dysfunction: Risks for the elderly?. Encephale 2009;35:361-9.  Back to cited text no. 35
    



 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1]



 

Top
  
 
  Search
 
  
    Access Statistics
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  

 
  In this article
    Abstract
    Materials and Me...
   Results
   Discussion
   Conclusion
    References
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed1005    
    Printed28    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded32    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal